Wason’s
Task and Confirmation Bias
Chris Duerschner
Confirmation
bias shows up pretty much wherever you find opinions. Some would even say that opinions are only
possible because of confirmation bias, or at least a precursor to it. The basic gist of the concept is that people
tend to filter out those things that conflict with their convictions and only
perceive those that reinforce them. The
focus of this post is not, however, to explore the extent to which confirmation
bias occurs or to detail its underlying mechanism, but to explain the
experimental evidence that shows it does occur.
Because confirmation bias is such a simple event; it makes sense that
the most well known study that demonstrates it is also simple.
This study has become known colloquially as
Wason’s 2-4-6 Task. The task focuses on
how people generate hypotheses. Its
general aim is to “discover an underlying rule that specifies the relationship
between three numbers.” (J. Russo, M. Meloy).
For example, for the three numbers used in the test (2, 4, 6) the rule
might be even numbers, numbers separated by 2, or a variety of other
categories. The subject could formulate
hypotheses and test them by putting forward a series of numbers that
demonstrated it and receiving a yes or no answer. The simplicity of the task is misleading as
most people’s final hypothesis is incorrect.
To explain these results, Wason noticed that participants typically
sought positive feedback and would grow increasingly confident in their
hypotheses after several consecutive yeses had been obtained. Few, however, would attempt to falsify their
hypotheses, causing most to end up with an incorrect conclusion.
For
example, given the initial sequence 2-4-6, a participant might formulate the
hypothesis “even numbers”. They might
test this by putting forward the sequences 8-10-12 (response yes), and then 14-2-20
(response no). They might then revise
their hypothesis to be increasing even numbers and test it with the sequences
4-6-8 (response yes), 10-18-26 (response no).
They would then revise again to make the hypothesis consecutive
increasing even numbers, and test with 12-14-16, 16-18-20, 32, 34-36 (responses
all yes) until they felt sufficiently confident that they had discovered the
pattern. Unfortunately the true pattern
is simply increasing numbers. As you may
have noticed in the example, it was after exceptions to the rule had been
discovered that the participant’s hypotheses were revised, and if the
participant had continued to seek falsification, rather than verification, they
might have eventually discovered the correct rule. Indeed in the example, I was overly kind to
the participant, as in the actual trials most participants never eliminated a
single hypothesis.
The conclusions drawn from this
study are broad and far-reaching. People naturally tend to seek evidence that
they are right and seldom go out of their way to look for evidence that they
are wrong. In social situations in which
the issues on the line are more important than a simple series of numbers, this
may be because individuals are constantly assessing the costs associated with
being wrong and modeling their attitudes and behaviors accordingly; in other
words they are formulating a so called path of least resistance for themselves. When put in terms of polarization, this
explains many different aspects of current behavior on the internet, such as the
overall popularity of Hess’s idiot opponent montages, the relative hemophilia throughout
its echo chambers and the selection that is exhibited in the media that an
individual experiences online.
No comments:
Post a Comment